Have you ever looked at someone’s face and said, for example, “You really have the face of Giovanni!“? Well, you should know that a 2024 study, conducted by the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, has actually investigated the possible correspondence between people’s first names and their facial appearance. In other words, the researchers wondered if there really is a tendency for people to “look like” the name they were given at birth. Apparently the answer is partly yes, so the team’s main goal was to understand the origins of this phenomenon: is it the result of an innate trait present from birth or is it something that develops gradually over the course of life due to social influence?
The authors of the study hypothesized that the correspondence between name and face is not innate, but the result of a self-fulfilling prophecyor a process through which people, consciously or unconsciously, adopt physical and behavioral characteristics that correspond to the socio-cultural stereotypes unconsciously associated with their name. This mechanism would suggest that socio-cultural expectations relating to how a person is expected to appear by virtue of his or her name influence the development of his or her appearance (we are talking about aesthetic choices such as hairstyles or glasses, facial expressions and behaviors) that would shape the facial appearance based on the expectations associated with the name.
Self-fulfilling prophecy or innate correspondence?
The research wanted to test two hypotheses starting points relating to the potential correspondence between face and associated first name:
- Self-fulfilling prophecy: People’s facial appearance would evolve to fit the stereotypes associated with their name as they age and are exposed to social interactions.
- Innate correspondence: a person’s name would be chosen based on his or her appearance (actual or predicted) from birth, suggesting that a congruence between face and name is evident even in childhood.
Methods and approaches used
The study uses three distinct methodologies to test these two starting hypotheses:
- Face-name matching by human participants: In this phase, both adults and children were asked to complete an experiment in which they had to match the faces of people with their names, both for adults and children. Participants had to select the correct name of a person based on the photo of the person, with four possible name options for each face. The researchers predicted that the ability to match faces and names would be better for adults’ faces than for children’s faces, since stereotypes related to names are consolidated in adulthood.
- Machine learning and facial similarity analysis: A machine learning algorithm was used to analyze facial similarity between individuals with the same name. The goal was to determine whether facial representations of adults with the same name were more similar than those of children with the same name. This approach provided an objective analysis free from human bias, thus increasing the validity of the results and reducing the possibility of error resulting from unconscious biases of the participants.
- Artificial aging of children’s faces: To further test the role of social influences and distinguish the biological from the social aging process, the researchers used algorithms GAN to digitally age images of children, turning them into adult versions. Then, both human participants and machine learning assessed whether these artificially aged faces resembled their names, to see whether the development of face-name correspondence was linked only to biological aging or also to social influences.
Main results compared to the methods used
- Face-Name Matching: adults demonstrated a greater ability to correctly match names to faces of other adults, with results significantly above chance. However, when it came to children, both adults and children themselves were unable to match names to faces accurately, suggesting that face-name correspondence does not occur in children.
- Machine learning: The algorithm found that facial representations of adults with the same name were significantly more similar than those of adults with different names. This pattern was not observed in children, reinforcing the idea that face-name correspondence develops with age and is not innate. This provides strong evidence for the self-fulfilling prophecy hypothesis.
- Artificial aging: images of children’s faces were digitally aged to make them appear like adults. These artificial faces did not show the same face-name correspondence as real adults. This confirms that the face-name correspondence effect It is not due exclusively to biological agingbut it would also require a process of social adaptation involving expectations related to the name.
Conclusions of the study
The results of the study support the hypothesis of the self-fulfilling prophecy. The facial appearance of individuals, particularly in adulthood, appears to conform to unconscious socio-cultural stereotypes associated with their names. However, This phenomenon is not present in childrensuggesting that the correspondence between face and name develops gradually over the course of life, rather than being innate from birth.
The similarity between face and name is therefore influenced by socio-cultural and behavioral expectations that manifest themselves over time. Factors such as hairstyle, makeup, and facial expression help shape a person’s face to reflect the stereotype associated with their name. This phenomenon, according to the authors, demonstrates the power of social influences even in the physical appearance of individuals, an area that is usually thought to be determined solely by genetic or biological factors.
Implications and Future Prospects
The study opens new perspectives on how social stereotypes influence not only the behavior and personality of individuals, but also their external appearance. Some areas of interest for future research include:
- Critical age: identify the precise age at which a person begins to resemble their name and understand how social expectations and experiences shape this process.
- Parental influence: to explore whether parents play an active role in choosing names that match their newborns’ appearance and whether there are differences between children named before birth and those named after birth.
- Types of names and stereotypes: to investigate whether certain names generate stronger stereotypes and whether there are categories of names that facilitate face-name correspondence more than others.